Skip main navigation

Functional equivalence

How does mental practice help athletes improve their skills? In this article, Dr Ben Marshall discusses how mental practice affects the brain.
A model section of a brain

Many studies have shown us that mental practice can be very effective for improving performance, but how and why does it work? There are a few different ideas but neuropsychology (the study of how the brain and nervous system influence a person’s behaviour) seems to make the strongest argument.

From previous research, we already know that imagery of an action shares many of the same properties as its physical performance.1 For example, imaged tasks tend to take the same amount of time to complete as when they are physically performed and the size and position of objects associated with the action are preserved. Developments in neuroimaging technology over the last 20 years have also allowed researchers to see what is happening inside the brain during mental practice when an individual performs an action. Examples of modern neuroimaging technology include functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) systems that show how blood flows around the brain in real time and electroencephalography machines that show the different levels of electrical activity across the brain.

fMRI Machine fMRI Machine,Wikimedia Commons (EconomicsUZH)

fMRI Output fMRI Output, flickr (daemonv)

EEG Setup EEG Setup, flickr (Cullen College)

EEG Output EEG Output, Dr Chesney Craig (Manchester Metropolitan University)

Using this technology, we are able to study which brain areas are active during imagery, action observation, and physical performance of an action. The results of various neuroimaging studies tell us that when an individual physically performs an action, areas of the brain that are associated with movement planning and control become more active, forming a distinctive pattern of neural activation.2 What is very interesting, however, is that many of the same areas also become active during mental practice.3

Hardwick brain images Hardwick Brain Images3

This evidence indicates that mental practice can be used to retain the neural circuits that are involved in a given movement active, even without physical practice.4 This is particularly important as it means the neural connections associated with a movement can be maintained, even during periods of physical inactivity (e.g. injury).

This similarity between the patterns of brain activation during mental practice and physical performance is known as functional equivalence. Therefore, in order for mental practice to be as effective as possible it should be as similar as possible to physical performance.

How can we try to make mental practice similar to physical performance? What do you think would need to be included in order to maximise functional equivalence?

References

  1. Decety J, Jeannerod M. Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt’s law hold in motor imagery?. Behavioural brain research. 1995;72(1-2):127-134.
  2. Lotze M, Montoya P, Erb M, Hülsmann E, Flor H, Klose U, Birbaumer N, Grodd W. Activation of cortical and cerebellar motor areas during executed and imagined hand movements: an fMRI study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 1999;11(5):491-501.
  3. Hardwick RM, Rottschy C, Miall RC, Eickhoff SB. A quantitative meta-analysis and review of motor learning in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2013;67:283-297.
  4. Mulder T. Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation. Journal of neural transmission. 2007;114(10):1265-1278.
This article is from the free online

Mental Skills Training for Sport and Health

Created by
FutureLearn - Learning For Life

Reach your personal and professional goals

Unlock access to hundreds of expert online courses and degrees from top universities and educators to gain accredited qualifications and professional CV-building certificates.

Join over 18 million learners to launch, switch or build upon your career, all at your own pace, across a wide range of topic areas.

Start Learning now