Skip to 0 minutes and 5 secondsSo hello everyone, and welcome to week 5, The Mind is Flat-- The Round Up. Jess, what are the issues in week 5? So in week 5, we've moved away a bit from the very individualistic perspective of the mind and started talking a bit more about coordination and how people work together in groups. And you kind of introduce this notion of we thinking and talk about how this means that we can perhaps create this incredibly complex and intelligent society, even if all of the individual minds within it are not-- well, certainly are complex as we said, but are not necessary kind of as deep as we might think.

Skip to 0 minutes and 38 secondsAnd you talk a lot about this human ability to coordinate and to think in groups and suggest that perhaps this is kind of an incredible thing that sets humans apart from other animals. But it seems like isn't it the case that other animals do certainly coordinate and cooperate in various ways? I think that a lot of people might be thinking you look at these animals like wolves who hunt in packs, or ants who do things kind of amazingly together in colonies. Could you perhaps say a bit about what you think is different about the human ability to coordinate? What exactly do you mean by we thinking and how that's different from what animals do? Yeah. Yeah.

Skip to 1 minute and 13 secondsSo first of all, I should say that this is a speculative claim, although not a totally incredible claim, but it is a speculative claim that there's something unique about human cognition in relation to being able to work together. The second thing is that it's not a claim-- the claim about we thinking is a claim about a way in which we're able to coordinate with our behaviour. It's not saying that that's the only way coordination can happen. So for example, termites are tremendously good coordinators. They don't think about each other's minds. In fact, they have very, very small minds and they certainly probably have no conception at all that other termites have any mind at all.

Skip to 1 minute and 49 secondsBut they are preprogrammed to follow simple rules, which when you put lots of termites together means that they lay trails and follow trails and so on, which creates these incredibly complicated results, a bit like the individual cells in your body are programmed to operate with each other without any kind of thought of the sort in a way that leads to incredibly complicated and effective entire physiological system. So it's not the case that mere coordination and mere complexity implies that we thinking or anything like it is going on. And similarly, thinking about something like wolves, then there's probably a large amount of biological preprogramming going on there too.

Skip to 2 minutes and 33 secondsNow with humans, the thing that's remarkable about us is we're able to work together to deal with totally novel problems. So when you give someone the problem of, say, assembling Ikea furniture, which is a pretty hard problem, and tell them they can't talk, it still turns out to be possible for people quite effectively to work out how to do this together. And people will do things like pick up a screw and look at the other person, and they'll hand over a screwdriver. And then when there's a heavy thing to lift, one person might start lifting it. The other will rush in and help.

Skip to 3 minutes and 6 secondsAnd people are actually very well able to do quite complicated, and clearly very evolutionarily novel tasks, without direct communication. And I think to do that requires that we're able to think not what am I trying to do and what are you trying to do, because if I think about that, I get into this kind of loop in which case where I think now, you've got a screw in your hand. I wonder what you're going to do with it. If you wanted to screw it into that board, I guess I ought to give you the screwdriver. But I don't know whether that's what you want to do.

Skip to 3 minutes and 37 secondsAnd of course you with the screw will be thinking, well, there's only any point me getting this screw and lining up if the person's going to give me the screwdriver. But are they? So you're in this kind of deadlock where each individual person's behaviour only makes sense if they know what the other person's going to do. But as soon as you think no, don't worry about what the other person's going to do who's thinking about where you're going to do in some kind of infinite loop, just think, what are we trying to achieve here?

Skip to 4 minutes and 1 secondAnd if we know that we're trying to assemble this furniture and we know that it requires a screwdriver into the screw or lifting a heavy piece of wood, then we know that to do those tasks requires coordination between two people and we'll just do it automatically. So I think the fact that we can think about what the team is trying to achieve very naturally, so naturally we don't even notice ourselves doing it, that's really crucial. There's no sign, at least on my fairly admittedly partial knowledge of the literature that there's no sign of other animals being able to do in a flexible way for novel problems in the way that we can.

Skip to 4 minutes and 35 secondsBut of course, that's crucial if you're going to, for example, solve difficult new problems and create new types of artefact and reproduce them if you're going to create organisations, if you're going to create forms of art, and so on and so on. And I think one thing to comment on there is that our ability to do this is so natural to us that we almost don't really see it as being remarkable. You talk about this example of the screw and the hammer and creating a table. I think that my instinctive reaction to that and probably a lot of other people's instinctive reaction is like, that's not amazing. Like, what? That's a very simple task.

Skip to 5 minutes and 9 secondsBut probably there are a few things to clarify there from what you said, and one is this idea of we thinking, as you said, is not synonymous with coordination. It's a specific kind of coordination, and it's coordination which is based on the ability to think not just about what other people are doing, but like what we would do if we could communicate and decide as a team. So sort of think as a team. And so that's the thing you're suggesting that humans can do and that animals can't. And then the other thing that you touched upon was this idea that for most animals, at least it seems, that the basis of most of their coordination is genetic.

Skip to 5 minutes and 45 secondsSo kind of like ants are preprogrammed to work together in certain ways. Wolves kind of have this genetic basis for certain kinds of coordination, perhaps because the ability to coordinate was very evolutionarily adaptive and so would've evolved. And I think the point that you're making is that because humans have this ability to coordinate beyond what is genetically programmed because we can think about what other people are thinking and think about what we would do as a team, that makes our ability to coordinate very flexible, and means that we can then coordinate in new, unknown situations that weren't necessarily genetically programmed, and that's what gives us the advantage. Is that an accurate summary of what you've been saying? Absolutely.

Skip to 6 minutes and 21 secondsAnd a good example, which we talk about this week, is the high low game. So the high low game's the game where we both shout high or low, and we get a big prize if we both shout high, a small one if we both shout low. There, it's completely obvious we're both going to shout high, but if I'm thinking about you thinking about me, then my thought is going to be I better do the same as you. So if you're going to shout low, I better shout low. If you're going to shout high, I better shout high. So I'll do the same as you, but you're trying to do the same as me. But what am I trying to do?

Skip to 6 minutes and 47 secondsI don't know. So we're going around in an infinite loop. Whereas if you immediately think, well what would we decide to do? It's obvious we'd decide to shout high. And that step to what should we do, is as you say, it's so transparent to us. It's so much the way we're built to think that we just don't notice it's profound and clever. But it really is, and it may be one of the secrets of human social behaviour.

Skip to 7 minutes and 9 secondsAnd I think again, like the high low game just sounds kind of-- it's in some ways, it's difficult to grasp the complexity of what you're saying with that and the fact that this is a complex thing, because it is just so obvious to us that it's like, well obviously we would both shout high. But I think the fact that it's obvious in a way is testament to the fact that this is like a very easy thing for us to do. And I think going on to next week, we will talk about obviously this high low game, the ability to do that.

Skip to 7 minutes and 33 secondsA bit like we talked with the money pump, it's a very abstract example and it doesn't seem-- it's kind of hard to see, OK, how is this useful in real life? But I think in next week's content, we're going to talk a bit more about how this ability to we think actually allows us to develop language and culture and society, and that should hopefully shed a bit more light on why this is actually relevant and important. So on that note, we'll stop for now, and we'll see you for the final week next week.

Talking Point: Week 5

This week we have considered the idea that there is a special type of thinking that allows people to engage in complex coordinated activities, including communication: ’we-thinking’.

In this video I talk with Jess about the common themes of the fifth week, Jess has also summarised the weeks themes here.

Talking Point

Before we move to the sixth and final week we’d be really interested to know how you’re finding the course this week, so please leave a comment or share some part of your experience so far in the discussion below:

  • If you’d heard of the ‘hi-lo’ game before, has your understanding of it changed after hearing about it in the context of this course?
  • Is ‘we-thinking’ really crucial to the construction of human culture and society?
  • How did you find this weeks experiments? How accurate (or not) were your predictions of other people’s answers?

On a scale of 1 to 10 (where 10 is a ‘believer’ and 1 is not) how much do you now subscribe to the idea of a ‘flat’ mind? It’ll be interesting to compare your answers here with previous weeks and see if anything has changed in either your understanding or appreciation of the idea or concept of a flat mind.

Don’t forget to contribute to the discussion by reviewing comments made by other learners, making sure you provide constructive feedback and commentary. You can also ‘like’ comments or follow other learners throughout the course.

Next Week

Next week, our last together, we will step up another level of complexity, focusing not on individuals (as in earlier weeks), nor on the coordination between individuals, but on the origin of some of the complex patterns that we collectively create: language, culture, and society.

Nick

Share this video:

This video is from the free online course:

The Mind is Flat: the Shocking Shallowness of Human Psychology

The University of Warwick

Get a taste of this course

Find out what this course is like by previewing some of the course steps before you join: