Skip main navigation

Investigating the Mozart Effect

Some of the studies that investigated the Mozart Effect
Hands holding magnifying glasses investigating a sheet of music
© University of Padova

Soon after Rausher and colleagues (1993) published the original experiment on Nature, the scientific community started to work to understand why that exceptional result was observed.

By 1999, it was already clear that the Mozart effect originally observed by Rausher and colleagues was likely a mirage: the replications that were carried out to check that original finding reported either a very small effect (much smaller than the original one) or no effect at all (e.g., see Steele, Bass, & Crook, 1999).

A Type I error?

In 1999, the journal Nature published two studies (including one meta analysis) that could put an end to the story (see Chabris, 1999; Steele et al., 1999). Meta analyses are studies in which scientists compare the results of various experiments investigating the same thing. From this meta analysis, it was clear that the original result observed by Rausher and colleagues was something like a “lucky find”. Note that the original authors were never questioned for fraud: they indeed observed that result.

But it seemed that they could have observed what is commonly described as a Type I error, which occurs when a result observed in a study isn’t actually a reflection of reality, and it’s a typical error in empirical research.

Every research and study is limited in space and time and it’s possible that the results gathered by one study don’t reflect the reality of the world.
For example, the observation that the music of Mozart makes you more intelligent could have no bearing outside the laboratory or research setting.

Studies that affirmed the original

However, as mentioned earlier, around the same time, the results of some studies would support the original finding, observing a Mozart effect (although smaller in size). There arose the question of why this positive effect of music could sometimes be observed.

Type of music

To answer this, the positive effect of Mozart’s music in particular needed to be investigated—was it just his music or could other types of music be used? The vinyl played by Rausher and colleagues included music by Schubert on its B-side (the fantasia Op. 103, D.940). Nantais and Schellenberg (1999) compared the effect of Mozart with the effect of Schubert. They observed that other types of classic music (not only Mozart’s) could be used to induce the Mozart effect.

Investigating the context

Another thing that required investigation was whether the effect was caused by music or could have been due to something else. One of the major problems of the first studies on the Mozart effect was that participants were often asked to either relax or sit in silence for ten minutes (two control conditions that were later compared with the music condition). If you sit in silence for ten minutes, you immediately realize how boring it can be. So in a 1999 study, Nantais and Schellenberg decided to drop the silence condition and instead played an audio book by Stephen King.
Participants that liked the audio book had a Mozart effect after listening to the book, whereas participants that liked Mozart revealed a Mozart effect after listening to the music by Mozart.
Thompson et al. (2001) investigated the characteristics that a stimulus required to obtain the Mozart effect. They compared the effect of the music by Mozart with the effect of one of Albinoni’s adagios, a typical composition with a slow tempo and is often played at funerals. The music by Mozart boosted cognitive performance whereas Albinoni’s did not.

Coming to a conclusion

Authors concluded that the music stimulus had to be happy and cheerful, something that could lift your mood. Finally, it’s clear that not everybody likes Mozart. For example, if you were a teenager, you’d rather listen to pop music than good old Mozart.
Schellenberg and Hallam (2006) compared the effect of the music by Mozart with a song from the band Blur, which, at the time, was a number one hit in the UK charts. In this study, the Mozart effect was observed with the music of the Blur and not with the music of Mozart, thereby demonstrating that everybody has his own taste.
In another experiment (Nakata, Hunter, & Tamoto, 2007), two to five year old Japanese children showed a Mozart effect after listening to children’s music and not after listening to the music of the Austrian genius Schellenberg (i.e. these children were young, and the performance they were asked to do was draw).
All in all, the decades of studies that investigated the Mozart effect have allowed us to shed some light on the original finding observed by Rauscher et al. (1993).

References

  • Bangerter, A., & Heath, C. (2004). The Mozart effect: Tracking the evolution of a scientific legend. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43(4), 605-623.
  • Chabris, C. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’?. Nature 400, 826–827.
  • Nantais, K. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1999). The Mozart effect: An artifact of preference. Psychological Science, 10(4), 370-373.
  • Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C. N. (1993). Music and spatial task performance. Nature, 365(6447), 611-611.
  • Rauscher, F. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’? Nature 400, 827–828.
  • Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2001). Arousal, mood, and the Mozart effect. Psychological science, 12(3), 248-251.
  • Schellenberg, E., & Hallam, S. (2006). Music listening and cognitive abilities in 10 and 11 year-olds: The blur effect. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 202-209.
  • Schellenberg, E. G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P. G., & Tamoto, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cognitive performance: Tests of children and adults. Psychology of music, 35(1), 5-19.
  • Steele, K. M., Bass, K. E., & Crook, M. D. (1999). The mystery of the Mozart effect: Failure to replicate. Psychological Science, 10(4), 366-369.
  • Steele, K., Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I. et al. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’?. Nature 400, 827.
© University of Padova
This article is from the free online

Music and Intelligence: Can Music Make You Smarter?

Created by
FutureLearn - Learning For Life

Reach your personal and professional goals

Unlock access to hundreds of expert online courses and degrees from top universities and educators to gain accredited qualifications and professional CV-building certificates.

Join over 18 million learners to launch, switch or build upon your career, all at your own pace, across a wide range of topic areas.

Start Learning now