Skip main navigation

New lower prices! Get up to 50% off 1000s of courses. 

Explore courses

Music’s journey through the brain?

Music doesn’t just exist. We perceive music because we turn vibrations of the air into conscious experience. So how does that happen?
3D illustration of ear anatomy with music or sound coming in
© Shutterstock

Music doesn’t just exist. We perceive music because we turn vibrations of the air into conscious experience. So how does that happen?

At a very basic level, the music that we hear, whether through headphones or through loudspeakers at a concert, is made up of vibrations of the air. This is all a loudspeaker (or a piano) does: it makes the air vibrate (Thompson, 2015).

We’re descended from animals who found it useful to know when the air vibrated around them, as it might usefully let you know that there is a snarling carnivore nearby trying to eat you.

Converting vibrations of the air to music

So when you listen to music, something very basic has to happen: you need to convert vibrations of the air into something that makes sense. Is that air vibration because of a lion? Or is it just the gong at the end of ‘Bohemian Rhapsody’?

There are two parts to this conversion: sensation and perception. For psychologists, sensation is the process of converting a physical signal (for example, a vibration of the air) into a neural signal (neurons sending electrochemical impulses to each other).

This is, fundamentally, what your ear does: it channels and amplifies sounds into a snail-shaped organ in the ear called the ‘cochlea’.

Different ‘hair cells’ of the cochlea detect higher or lower-pitched sounds (e.g., Freddie Mercury’s falsetto vs John Deacon’s bass guitar), and this detection triggers an electrical signal that gets sent to the brain. If you experience a ringing sound in your head after a concert, part of the reason for it is because of damage to hair cells in the cochlea (Wilson, 1980); because it’s easier to damage the parts of the cochlea that detect higher-pitched noises. This is why tinnitus (a constant ringing in the ears that some people experience) is often high-pitched.

Perceiving

In contrast to sensation, perception is the process of your brain analysing those neural signals coming from the ear, which turns them from a bunch of electricity into something that you experience as sounds (including musical sounds).

Your brain is made up of billions of cells called neurons, which send electrochemical signals to each other, and which are organised within the brain in particular ways. Research suggests that large parts of a part of your brain called the ‘temporal lobes of the cortex’ are devoted to analysing different parts of sounds as they come in. In the image below you can see this part of the brain, highlighted in orange, which is physically just behind the ears.

A 3D stylised front and profile view of the head highlighting, in orange, the temporal cortex.

One part of the temporal cortex analyses the difference between different vowels in speech, while another part analyses the difference between rhythms in music.

If your temporal cortex becomes damaged, for example if you are in a car accident and something hits your head, you may find that you experience ‘amusia’ (Peretz, Champod, & Hyde, 2003), a condition where people find it difficult to tell the difference between different musical notes.

For instance, someone with amusia might find that musical sounds of different pitches all sound the same; one person with amusia has said that the (quite beautiful and relaxing) piano music of the composer Frederic Chopin sounds to her like a lot of plates smashing.

Your task

You might be aware that dogs can hear a different range of frequencies to us. Famously there are dog whistles that make sounds that dogs can hear but we cannot.

Thinking about the process of sensation and perception described above, why must a dog be able to hear those sounds but we cannot?

Use the comment link below to discuss your thoughts with other learners.

References

Peretz, I., Champod, A. S., & Hyde, K. (2003). Varieties of musical disorders: the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 58-75. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.006

Thompson, W. F. (2015). Music, Thought, and Feeling: Understanding the Psychology of Music (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Wilson, J. P. (1980). Evidence for a cochlear origin for acoustic re-emissions, threshold fine-structure and tonal tinnitus. Hearing Research, 2, 233-252. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-5955(80)90060-X

© Griffith University
This article is from the free online

Music Psychology: Why Does "Bohemian Rhapsody" Feel so Good?

Created by
FutureLearn - Learning For Life

Reach your personal and professional goals

Unlock access to hundreds of expert online courses and degrees from top universities and educators to gain accredited qualifications and professional CV-building certificates.

Join over 18 million learners to launch, switch or build upon your career, all at your own pace, across a wide range of topic areas.

Start Learning now