Skip main navigation

New offer! Get 30% off one whole year of Unlimited learning. Subscribe for just £249.99 £174.99. New subscribers only T&Cs apply

Find out more

What consciousness is for

What is consciousness for? Watch as Mark Solms talks about how feelings relate to consciousness and instinctual drive to survive and reproduce.
10
Identifying the part of the brain that’s necessary for consciousness is really powerful knowledge. By studying that part of the brain physiologically and anatomically, looking, for example, to what it’s connected to, we can ask deeper questions about the mind. We can ask, for example, what is consciousness for? Why do we have consciousness at all? This is a really basic question about, ultimately, what the mind is for, because I’m arguing that consciousness is one of the two defining properties that I’ve introduced so far as to what the essential ingredients or constituents or components of a mind are.
55.7
If we look at the reticular activating system, which is the part of the brain that’s essential for consciousness, we observe something really important about it. One of the many structures which make up the reticular activating system is called the periaqueductal grey. This is the smallest part of the brain, which, when damaged, leads to a loss of consciousness. It is, in a way, the most condensed consciousness generating piece of anatomy that there is. Remember, all vertebrates have a periaqueductal grey. This is a structure which is about 525 million years old. This, in humans, is about the size of a jelly bean. Think of it.
99.9
You damage this part of your brain– yours or mine– the size of a jelly bean– and you are gone. All consciousness is obliterated. Let me tell you some of the things we’ve learned about the periaqueductal grey. The most important thing is that if you stimulate the dorsal columns– the back part of the periaqueductal grey– it generates not just consciousness, but very unpleasant consciousness, pain, distress. The person in whom this part of the brain is being stimulated or any other animal wants nothing more than for you to stop the stimulation, and they want to get away from you.
137.3
Conversely, the front part, the ventral columns of the periaqueductal grey, when that part is stimulated, exquisitely, delicious, delightful, orgasmic sensations of consciousness are generated.
154.1
When you stimulate this part of the animal, it really likes you and wants you to carry on. These are the sorts of predictions that one makes. In humans, we know that it does this. It causes feelings of pleasure in the front and feelings of unpleasure in the back. So we predict that if we stimulate the front part in other vertebrates, they’re going to want us to carry on. And conversely, the back part, they’re going to want to get away from us. That prediction is confirmed so we provisionlly hold to the conclusion that the periaqueductal grey does in those creatures the same thing that it does in us.
189.2
The important point that I’m wanting to make is that this condensed, most central consciousness generating structure in the brain doesn’t just switch on the lights. It’s not just a quantitative turning up the volume of consciousness. It doesn’t just make you awake. Being awake feels like something. There’s an emotional tone to it. There always is. Pleasurable or unpleasurable states accompany all of consciousness. In fact, this is the conclusion that we’ve reached in recent years on the basis of research on this part of the brain, principally by Antonio Damasio and Jaak Panksepp. We’ve reached the conclusion that the essential characteristic of consciousness is feeling, emotion. Emotion doesn’t come from our eyes, and our ears, and our noses. It comes from within us.
242.2
Feelings, the core of consciousness are about us. They’re subjective.
250.3
Now we can go further. The periaqueductal grey and the whole of the reticular activating system– all of which has this feeling tone attached to it– just the periaqueductal grey that’s the sort of most intense version of that truth about the reticular activating system. It, in turn, is activated by other nuclei in the brainstem, which you’re looking at now. These nuclei have one thing in common. They all monitor the state of the body, the internal milieu of the body, how you’re doing in terms of heartbeat, breathing rate, sugar levels, salt levels, core body temperature, and so on. It’s absolutely crucial for your survival that these parameters stay within a very narrow range.
298.9
And those brain structures are busy monitoring and making sure that you are staying within that range. The summation of what they’re finding out is broadcast to the reticular activating system as feelings. That seems to be what feelings are for. Feelings tell you how you’re doing within this biological scale of values. If you feel good, that means carry on doing this, because this is good for your survival, and as it happens, your reproductive success. If it feels bad, stop doing it, because it’s bad for your chances of surviving and reproducing. So when we say that there was a dawn of consciousness 525 million years ago, this is what consciousness added to what the rest of the body was capable of doing.
346.1
It enables the creature to– us included– to know how we’re doing biologically speaking. And please note, it’s connected to the interior of my body, which is an explanation for why consciousness is subjective. It’s about to me. It’s about how I’m doing. After that, the consciousness is broadcast upwards to the rest of the brain. That’s a second order of consciousness. I feel like this about that. These are the sorts of things that we’re learning about consciousness. By identifying the brain mechanisms related to it, these are the things that we’re learning about what the mind is all about and what the mind is for. It also explains something about qualia. It explains why your qualia are subjective– why I can’t see yours.
400.2
It’s because your endogenous consciousness activates your perceptual apparatus. So it’s something from inside of you that’s sampling the perceptions. They have to be subjective.

Being conscious is about more than being awake, it also feels like something to be awake.

And these feelings give us a clue about what consciousness is for. The reticular activating system is the brain structure responsible for consciousness. It is connected to and activated by other brain structures that monitor your vital signs – such as heart rate, temperature and glucose levels. These brain structures are called ‘need detectors’ and they convey the state of your body through feelings. These feelings tell the organism – you – how it is doing on a biological scale of values for surviving and reproducing. If it feels good, you are motivated to continue; if it feels bad, you want to stop.

Please note that I am not suggesting that you should do more of everything that feels good. What I am saying is that from an evolutionary biological perspective, feeling is the mechanism our consciousness uses to ensure the survival of our body and maximise our chances of reproducing. So when something is good for our survival, it feels good. However this does not mean that everything that feels good is good for our survival. That is why more complex mechanisms are also required. These will be introduced later.

Another important point about consciousness is that it is about you, your own body, how you are feeling. In other words, consciousness is subjective in nature.

This article is from the free online

What is a Mind?

Created by
FutureLearn - Learning For Life

Reach your personal and professional goals

Unlock access to hundreds of expert online courses and degrees from top universities and educators to gain accredited qualifications and professional CV-building certificates.

Join over 18 million learners to launch, switch or build upon your career, all at your own pace, across a wide range of topic areas.

Start Learning now